Delaware Review of Latin American Studies
Issues
Vol. 15 No. 2   December 31, 2014


Neighbors and Neighborhoods. The Effects of Proximity and Educational and Economic Status on Personal Networks in Argentina

Pablo De Grande
Instituto de Investigación en Ciencias Sociales (IDICSO)
Universidad del Salvador, Buenos Aires
pablodg@gmail.com



Acknowledgment
I would like to acknowledge research support from the Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas of Argentina (CONICET) and from the Observatorio de la Deuda Social (ODSA). I would also like to acknowledge the thoughtful, open and sustained help and guidance from Agustín Salvia all along this project. In addition, I would like to thank Manuel Eguia, who dedicated time and efforts introducing me to the social network analysis rationale and its literature with great cleverness and generosity. This manuscript also benefited from language corrections from Daniela Alcaraz.

Abstract
In this paper, we present results of a national (7 cities) personal network study carried out in Argentina during 2006. The relation between sociability and neighborhood is examined, stressing the idea that neighborhood unfolds as a complex, multidimensional phenomenon that spreads from past experience to present relations, creating new connections but also consolidating existing ones. Additionally, the interplay among economic status, educational status, personal ties and neighborhood sociability is explored, summarizing descriptive analysis of the information gathered after a name generator based survey.

Key words: Sociability, Economic status, Educational status, Neighborhood, Personal networks

******************************

.

I. Introduction

While in theoretical terms, social network analysis and personal network studies are bound to the classical sociological concerns of Simmel’s sociability (Simmel, 1949 [1910]), as well as Durkheim’s ‘social tie’ (Durkheim, 1984 [1893]:104), in practical terms personal network studies led interpersonal relations research to quantify and make explicit observation of ties and exchanges that had never been made before (Degenné y Forsé, 1999).

Personal network studies have focused on investigating a specific dimension of the broader concept of sociability: Everyday relations and interactions. Within such a domain, the link between everyday life and neighborhood soon emerged. Nevertheless, two restrictive patterns are often recognizable in these studies. First, the topic of neighborhood has often been assimilated into the problem of poverty, thus investigating ‘slums’ and low-income neighborhoods but excluding middle class and high-income neighborhoods (for comparison or specific research). Second, neighborhood was frequently defined as the social space of 'neighbors', excluding the relationships or experiences that occur in nearby homes to people not considered merely neighbors.

This article summarizes the information obtained through a personal network module in 7 large cities of Argentina. In 2006, we designed and applied a module (questionnaire) for personal ties as a household survey Encuesta de la Deuda Social Argentina (ODSA, 2007). The goals regarding the neighborhood and sociability that this module was designed to cover were:

1) Analyze the relationship between personal ties and neighborhood. Specifically, to estimate how relevant the neighborhood was today in people’s daily lives in large urban centers of Argentina.

2) Investigate the relevance of neighborhood through a multi-dimensional approach. That is, not to consider only the part of the neighborhood the people identified as 'neighbors', but also to look at the geographical distance of other people, as well as the current and past experiences relating to the neighborhood.

3) Explore the relationship between elements of social structure (economic capital, educational capital, age and gender) and the pervasiveness of personal ties within the neighborhood. We were interested in verifying assumptions about the relationship between poverty and neighborhood, and between residential isolation and poverty that were prevalent in the literature.

4) Following the separation of economic and cultural capital proposed by Pierre Bourdieu (Bourdieu, 1994), we wanted to test whether differences in the relation between personal networks and neighborhood could be identified for those types of capital.

An outline of the paper follows. Section II briefly reviews the literature bridging the neighborhood and personal relationships. Section III describes the characteristics of the survey as well as the sample and the variables used in the analysis. Section IV summarizes the information obtained by the survey about personal relations by educational status, economic status, age and gender, organized by type of personal tie, origin and geographical distance. Section V relates the initial goals to the evidence, discussing some theoretical and practical implications. 

II. Neighborhood and personal networks

Since the advent of the Chicago School concerns (Park et al., 1925; Wirth, 1928), and even before that (Engels, 1987 [1845]), neighborhood has been identified as a key concept for understanding daily life in the metropolitan landscapes. People inhabiting large cities often center many of their activities –from daily consumption to children’s education– within the limits of their neighborhood, reproducing both neighborhood features and typical local profiles.

However, even when the link between poverty and neighborhood was substantially sustained by empirical research (Wilson, 1987;  Jenks & Mayer, 1990; Ainsworth, 2002), Chicago school scholars have  been often criticized for treating each neighborhood as a closed (independent) social space, omitting forces that may influence the local space at the city, country and cross-country levels (Gravano, 2005).

The definition of the neighborhood as an open, multidimensional phenomenon, however, is still controversial (Hipp et al., 2012). While Schwirian identified in existing literature the usage of the neighborhood alternatively as a natural area, a social area and a system of interaction (Schwirian, 1983), Gravano suggests that the literature describes the three key dimensions of neighborhoods as neighborhood being a space (physical and administrative), a scene and a functional element (Gravano, 2003).

It’s worth noting as well that personal relations and the neighborhood have been linked through different approaches. Three of them are briefly summarized: Studies of residential segregation, studies on social capital, and studies on ‘networks’ (personal networks, social networks and social support networks).

First, the study of segregation has been often used to try to connect neighborhoods in terms of their urban integration (Duncan & Duncan, 1955; Cortese et al., 1976; Massey & Denton, 1988; Morgan, B. y Norbury, 1981; Goodman, 1985; Dawkins, 2004; White 1984; Wong 2002). Recent studies have updated the segregation map of many Latin American cities (Kaztman, 2001; Kaztman & Retamoso, 2005; Groisman & Suárez, 2005, 2006; Sabatini et al., 2001; Torres, 2009; Rodríguez & Arraigada, 2004; Salvia & De Grande, 2007). as poverty and inequality levels rose rapidly after neoliberal policies were implemented in the region (Sabaté, 2000; Kaztman, 1999; Salvia, 2001; CEPAL, 2001; CEPAL/CELADE, 2002). Segregation studies usually calculate coefficients –like Duncan’s segregation index (Duncan & Duncan, 1955)– to estimate (after census data) the levels of diversity in a population’s geographical distribution. As a problematic feature of segregation studies, it can be outlined that they mostly derive negative consequences of segregation for low-income districts, but are vague at analyzing the implications (positive or negative) of segregation configurations for middle class or high income neighborhoods. A significant strength of this approach is that it provides comparable isolation measures for any city where census data is available to process at the leve of a district, a census track, a block or any similar small-sized geographical unit. Despite this strength, a limiting factor of this type of information is that census data tend to monitor attribute data of the target population (occupational status, gender, age, and so on), thus making it difficult to incorporate relational information in the segregation arguments (such as the dynamics of the ties with family members and friends, daily interaction outside the neighborhood and so on).

Second, some of the literature on social capital literature has deepened the understanding of the connections between the interactions in the neighborhood and personal outcomes. Social capital has been investigated and criticized from different perspectives (Woolcock, 2001; De Filippis, 2001, ONS, 2001; Bagnasco et. al. 2004; Sabatini, 2003), organized in two wide areas of interest: institutional or community level social capital (usually related to trust and collective assets or capacities of a group), and personal social capital (focused on individual interactions and relations).

Among the latter, personal relations seen as social capital facilitating access to resources in the neighborhood have been examined since the 90’s with diverse results (PNUD, 1998; Burt, 2000; Lin, 2001, Van der Gaag, 2005; Atria et. al., 2003; Forni y Nardone, 2005; Sabatini, 2008). Organizations like CEPAL and the World Bank have found in social capital a powerful concept to incorporate into their analysis of the informal exchanges of goods and services that people mobilize to organize their daily needs in view of the lack of other economic resources (Grootaert, 1998; Lederman, 2001; Atria et al., 2003).

Finally, social support networks, personal networks and social networks studies also focused on the conditions and effects of socialization in everyday life. These studies investigate everyday interactions, thus often informing about neighborhood and community dynamics.

- Social support studies (similar to social capital) focus on the beneficial consequences of personal relations. However, while social capital is sometimes treated as a community level asset, social support refers to interpersonal relations, and specifically focuess on ways in which people help each other rather than giving an account of ‘resources through relations’ (House et al., 1988; Enriquez Rosas, 2000; Maya Jariego & Holgado, 2005; Mickelson & Kubzansky  2003; Agneessens et al., 2006; Lin et al., 1979; Lieber & Sandefur, 1998; Van der Poel, 1993).

- Personal network studies investigate methodological and substantive topics about how people create, maintain and use personal relations (Fischer, 1982; McCarty et al., 1997; Espinoza, 1999; Wellman y Potter, 1999; Grossetti, 2005; Ferrand et al. 1999; Lee et al. 2005; Molina, 2005; De Grande & Eguia, 2008). Personal network studies do not necessarily care about how useful those relations are, but rather concentrate on the dynamics and structure of personal relations. Typical aspects they describe are the origins and type of existing relations, and the interrelation of personal relations with more formal/institutional structures, as well as the standard network indicators and features in personal networks (size, degree, transitivity, centrality, betweennes).

- Social networks studies group together a wide variety of research, sometimes conceptually and empirically overlapping with social capital, social support and personal networks studies. However, the term ‘social network’ can refer to a wider range of relations, mapping the interactions between people and institutions, and describing large networks such as the Internet blogs, traffic dynamics or co-citation structures. Among those studies, the relevance of space (physical distance) has been outlined as a covariate, reinforcer or modifier for social space forces (Latané et al. 1995; Molina et al. 2012; Doreian & Conti, 2012). Consequently, people who are close to each other are  more likely to create relations (Preciado et al. 2012, Daraanova et al. 2012; Schaefer, 2012) that are more influential, and promote  participation  in activities in well-known places or where there are people they know well (Ioannides & Zabel, 2008). 

Social support, personal network and social network research often investigate aspects of sociability in substantive areas that do not rely on neighborhood as a crucial dimension, such as when studying health (Lin et al., 1979; Castro et al., 1997),  psychological well-being (Gencoz y Ozlale, 2004; Kenneth et al. 1978; Martínez García et al., 2002), friendship (Mcpherson et. al., 2001; De Federico de la Rúa, 2003; Stevens & Van Tilburg, 2011), access to labor markets (Granovetter, 1973; Feldman & Murmis, 2002) and political  behavior (Zuckerman, 2005; Szwarcberg, 2012).

This article empirically explores the connections among neighborhood and personal relations, linking distance to interactions, but also considering neighborhood as a functional (socially stratified) space and as a social scene for making acquaintances and socializing. 

III. Methodology

Survey sample

The information used in this article comes from the Encuesta de la Deuda Social Argentina (ODSA, 2007). This survey has been administered annually since 2004 in various large urban centers of Argentina to collect information on human development indicators1.

Households for the sample are selected using a stratified sampling procedure. Using the National Census demographic data and cartography of 2001, a set of 250 sample points were randomly chosen. Field representatives then visited each sample point to identify 6 addresses (houses / apartments) to look for respondents. Up to two visits were made if the respondent was not home; replacement addresses were provided when there were no eligible respondents or when they were not willing to answer the survey. Quotas of gender and age were followed to keep the sample proportional to each city’s known distribution.

Name generator

In 2006, a onetime module of personal networks was applied to all adults responding to the survey (n=1500). Similar to Burt’s General Social Survey module (Burt, 1984), this module was based on a single item name generator related to personal networks, with many items applied on each name enumerated.

The reasons for relying on a single-item for eliciting the names of the alteri followed Burt’s rationale for the General Social Suvey (1984, p. 322). In a household survey scenario, the fact that personal network modules (even using a single name generator) requires a lot of time to complet is crucial. As the name-generator technique gathers information for each name that is mentioned, the number of items grow rapidly as more contacts are elicited. Moreover, using a multiple-name generator strategy requires dealing with duplicated names during name elicitation, thus augmenting the complexity of the field and the chances for errors and misleading information.

The name generator item was:

Often, people turn to friends, family, co-workers or acquaintances when they need advice or help in situations that be difficult to resolve without them. Among your acquaintances, not including those who live in your home, please, tell me only the first name of someone you depend on under such circumstances. (ODSA, 2006)2

The total number of names mentioned was registered, and then up to five names were investigated in terms of the characteristics of the ‘alter’ and the tie. The attributes gathered for the alteri were gender, age and education attainment. The attributes gathered for the tie were frequency of contact, duration, origin, type, distance from ego’s house to the alteri’s houses, content of the relationship (personal talk vs. non-personal talk) and level of knowledge among the alteri3.

During the study, a total of 1448 ties were collected and investigated (i.e. about one tie on average per person).

Analysis

During the analysis, the following variables were chosen to group the personal relations gathered by the name generator previously described:

- Educational status: Educational status is included both as a proxy for the position of the respondent in social stratification (cultural capital) and as an indicator of individual’s path through formal institutions. It is expected that educational experiences influence lifestyle preferences and goals, and also provide specific opportunities to socialize with others in the field (teachers, professors and students). Educational status is measured by the highest educational achievement of the respondent (ego). For data analysis, educational status has been grouped into three categories: Low, Medium and High.

Low: up to 7 years of education (only elementary school or less)
Medium: 8 to 12 years of education (i.e. high school studies)
High: more than 12 years of education (i.e. university or tertiary studies)

- Economic status: Economic status is relevant to the creation and maintenance of personal relations, not only because of the costs that may exist in taking care of specific aspects of the relationships, but also because economic status is linked to lifestyles (consumer habits, clubs, entertainment, holiday places) that by themselves guide exchanges within certain sorts of ‘class boundaries’. To identify the category of economic status for each respondent, we calculated the total monthly income per equivalent adult in its household4. Then, we grouped respondents in tertiles, being at the highest tertile those with income per equivalent adult above AR$610 (U$S 198) and at the lowest tertile those with income per equivalent adult below AR$285 (U$S 93).

- Age: Sociability is known to be largely dependent on the individual’s life cycle. As age correlates to participation in typical states and spaces of interaction (e.g. school, workplace, retirement), it is used in the analysis to control its effects in the sociability outcomes described. The variable used is the age of the respondent, grouped into three categories: 18-35 years, 36 -55 years, 56 years and more.

- Gender: Whether the respondent (the ‘ego’) is classified as male or female. In the XX century, women have progressively acquired rights leading toward equality in educational institutions and labor market participation. In 2001 in Argentina, 42.3% of the economically active population were women, while 57% of people taking university education were women5. However, gender remains a major dimension in organizing domestic and public activities, end-consumer products, labor market profiles, entertainment and –much related to all of them­– personal relations. As such, it is expected that gender may signal different behaviors regarding personal ties and neighborhood influence. The information was processed using the SPSS 13.0 package. Statistical significance of differences among categories was determined by t-tests (95% confidence). The resulting ‘p’ values are indicated inline through the analysis when differences proved significant (p < 0.01).

IV. Results

Summary

In Figure 1 a description of the sample is shown. Given the name generator used, the number of ties enumerated was rather low compared to previous studies, with an average of 0.97 ties per respondent. From the overall sample (n=1500), 45% of the respondents declared having no ties to look for when in need of help or advice. For the purpose of this article, where characteristics of personal ties are examined, only respondents declaring relations are considered in the analysis. The educational status, economic status and age variables have been grouped in order to ensure enough cases in all its categories, having similar size for all categories and yet retaining substantial meaning (i.e. low educational status means primary education or less).

Even when higher status is related to more personal ties available both for economic and educational status, in the case of educational status the number of personal ties per respondent shows the lowest and highest levels in the table, ranging from 0.68 for the Low group to 1.33 for High group.

Regarding age, the number of personal ties decline as people grow older, starting at 1.23 ties per respondent in the youngest group, falling to 0.72 ties per respondent for the 56-and-above group.

Figure 1. Summary of personal networks sample by educational status, economic status, age and gender. Argentina, 2006 (selected cities).
 

 

Respondents (n)

Respondents declaring no ties

Respondents
(N)

Personal ties (count)

Personal ties (average)

Personal ties (standard deviation)

Educational status

 

 

 

 

 

 

Low

560

302

3,956,622

383

0.68

0.93

Medium

543

237

3,831,772

537

0.99

1.15

High

397

139

2,803,596

528

1.33

1.37

Economic status

 

 

 

 

 

 

Low

500

249

3,532,661

397

0.79

1.05

Medium

500

230

3,531,773

469

0.94

1.14

High

500

199

3,527,556

582

1.16

1.27

Age

 

 

 

 

 

 

18-35

556

205

3,926,265

686

1.23

1.35

36-55

516

236

3,641,621

454

0.88

1.04

56+

428

237

3,024,103

309

0.72

0.98

Gender

 

 

 

 

 

 

Male

773

381

5,461,879

690

0.89

1.16

Female

727

297

5,130,110

758

1.04

1.17

Total

1500

678

10,591,990

1448

0.97

1.17

Source: Author’s calculations based on data from the Encuesta de la Deuda Social Argentina 2006.

Type of personal tie

The type of personal tie is related to the role structure established between the respondent (ego) and the persons enumerated by the respondent. It is expected that the role of a relationship (being friend, father, etc.) sets some guideline for the content, frequency and other characteristics of the relationship. The ‘neighbor’ category appears here as a tag for people who live or lived close to the respondent, not being signaled as s friend, mate, coworker or kin. The questionnaire offered a list of 9 possible roles (plus an ‘other’ category), which has been grouped into 6 categories in Figure 2 (plus an ‘Other’ and a ‘Non-response’ columns).

First, it’s worth pointing out that friendship is the more frequent type of tie declared, covering 60% of the personal ties enumerated. Under the figure of `friend', variations by educational status range from 54% to 63.8% (p < 0.005) and by economic status they range by 56.3% to 62.5% (Figure 2).

The greater changes by status are observed in the role of neighbor, which rises to 11.1% in the lower educational stratum from 2.1% (p < 0.001) in the higher educational stratum (similar variations are observed by economic status). Also significant is the fall of kin participation when educational status increases, ranging from 30.7% to 21.2% (p < 0.005), which is not replicated in kin participation along economic status.

The evolution by age shows complex variations, with an increase of the family and neighborhood components as people grow older, increasing from 19.3% to 32% (p < 0.001) and from 2.1% to 10.1% (p < 0.001) respectively. At the same time, friends show a maximum of 70.2% at younger age, decreasing around 52% after age of 56 (p < 0.001).

Regarding gender differences, women exhibit relationships more tied to their families, by opposition to men who seem more connected beyond family spaces. Men show more friendship bonds (65.7% of friends between men and 56.2% between the women, p < 0.001), and while 18.7% of the male ties are with relatives, women present 29.6% of their bonds reserved for family interactions (p < 0.001).

To sum up, even when neighbors were the third most mentioned type of tie, its participation of 5.3% over the total number of ties would suggest a low relevance of neighborhood in the urban scenarios investigated. However, as mentioned earlier, sociability in the neighborhood can include present and past experiences, and be related as well with many relational contents. In order to evaluate these connections, we will next to examine the role of neighborhood in the creation of personal ties.

Figure 2. Personal ties distribution for type of relation by educational status, economic status, age and gender. Argentina, 2006 (selected cities).


Type of relation (% per row)

Friend

Kin

Neighbor

Boyfriend/Girlfriend

Coworker/ classmate

Professional services

Other

NR

Educational status

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Low

54.0

30.7

11.1

1.2

1.9

0.0

0.4

0.7

Medium

62.4

23.1

4.2

2.5

6.7

0.3

0.4

0.5

High

63.8

21.2

2.1

3.6

5.6

2.0

0.7

1.0

Economic status

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Low

56.3

25.1

9.0

2.1

4.6

0.9

0.4

1.6

Medium

62.2

25.9

3.7

1.6

4.7

0.4

0.9

0.5

High

62.5

22.7

3.9

3.6

5.6

1.1

0.2

0.3

Age

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

18-35

70.2

19.3

2.1

3.3

3.7

0.7

0.6

0.0

36-55

51.7

26.9

6.7

2.0

9.0

1.6

0.5

1.6

56+

52.8

32.0

10.1

1.7

2.0

0.2

0.3

0.9

Gender

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Male

65.7a

18.7a

5.2

3.2

5.6

0.6

0.8

0.4

Female

56.2a

29.6a

5.4

2.0

4.5

1.1

0.3

1.0

Total

60.7

24.4

5.3

2.6

5.0

0.9

0.5

0.7

Source: Author’s calculations based on data from the Encuesta de la Deuda Social Argentina 2006.

Origin

The origin of personal ties was examined by asking the respondents how they met the person they had mentioned. A total of 8 possible ways were offered (plus an ‘other’ category), which have been grouped into 5 categories in Figure 3 (plus an ‘Other/Non-response’ column).

As Degenné et Forsé has pointed out, the idea that social relations are organized in circles is not new (1999: 55). Even when people can meet randomly on the street, or can introduce themselves individually one to another, the most usual way of connecting to people is by participating in existing ‘social circles’ which can vary in nature, ranging from schools to offices, from grocery stores to local community places.

In this analysis, the places that people declared as the more common sources for originating relations were grouped into a ‘places’ category, representing the social spaces or social circles identified as more influential to everyday relations. They are locations (social and physical spaces) that provide stable contexts for interactions, thus guiding and facilitating sociability.

People known ‘in the neighborhood’, workplaces or educational institution make up 56.4% of the ties, while 14.3% of the personal ties of the sample correspond to contacts through people they know well.

When observed by educational and economic status, higher status corresponds to higher participation of educational sociability in the origin of personal ties, in spite of socialization through the neighborhood. While socialization happened in 40% of the ties ‘in the neighborhood’ for the lower economic status strata, only 18.1% of the ties declare this origin in the higher status strata (p < 0.001).

As it was to be expected, this relation is stronger as the educational status increases, as it implies more exposure to direct forms of educative institutionalization. Sociability ‘in the neighborhood’ is at 13,7% in the lower stratum, while it reaches 41.5% in the higher stratum (p < 0.001, Figure 3).

With regard to the relation between age and neighborhood ties, the participation of such ties remains stable around 26% between the two first categories (18 to 35 years, and 36 to 55 years), and shows an increase of almost 10 percentage points in the 56 years and up category(p < 0.005).

The distributions by gender are different in ties originated at the family and at the neighborhood. Whereas for men the personal ties obtained in the neighborhood reach 32.8%, for women they occur in smaller proportion (24.6%, p < 0.005). This difference seems compensated by a greater level of participation of women in family circles as a source of relations (Figure 3).

Figure 3. Personal ties distribution for origin by educational status, economic status, age and gender. Argentina, 2006 (selected cities).


Origin of the
personal tie
(% per row)

 

Places

 

Referred

Direct

Other, NR

Elementary, high school or university

Workplace

Neighborhood

Through a friend, girlfriend, boyfriend or kin

A kin

Educational status

 

 

 

 

 

 

Low

1,1

7,2

41,5

13,8

32,0

4,5

Medium

13,9

11,7

33,8

13,9

21,2

5,6

High

35,2

9,2

13,7

15,2

20,7

5,9

Economic status

 

 

 

 

 

 

Low

10,3

7,7

40,0

10,9

26,3

4,8

Medium

14,3

9,0

31,5

15,3

23,8

6,1

High

27,0

11,5

18,1

15,8

22,3

5,3

Age

 

 

 

18-35

28,9

6,0

26,8

15,0

18,5

4,9

36-55

10,2

14,9

26,3

15,5

26,9

6,1

56+

6,7

10,0

35,4

11,2

31,4

5,4

Gender

 

 

 

Male

19,1

11,2

32,8

12,9

18,4

5,6

Female

17,5

8,2

24,6

15,6

28,9

5,2

Total

18,3

9,6

28,5

14,3

23,9

5,4

Source: Author’s calculations based on data from the Encuesta de la Deuda Social Argentina 2006.

Geographical distance

In addition to past experiences that led to the creation of personal ties (the previous way we introduced to track the relevance of neighborhood) we investigated the distance between the house of the respondent and the enumerated persons.

As the exact separation could not be easily calculated during the interview, and as its meaning could vary from city to city due to different transportation facilities, we evaluated the possibility of recording the amount of time it takes to go to the enumerated person’s house. However, there was no guarantee that the same method of transportation would be used to cover the distance (as is more common with traveling to work), thus producing more confusion on how to respond to the item. Finally we decided to state a metric scale, trying to determine whether both persons lived close enough to walk in a few minutes (up to 2km6), to take a bus or train, or whether they lived in different cities (more than 50km7 away). The resulting scale is shown in Figure 4, and its results can be described as follows.

As we had expected, higher levels of economic or educational status are associated with higher chances of keeping personal ties at greater distance. Relations are the product of past experiences, and it is not uncommon that remote experiences require economic resources and are often related to going someplace to be educated (e.g. high school, university).

In the case of the economic status, the personal ties that are located within the same city but at more than 20 blocks (2km) go from 25.4% to 43.4% (p < 0.001) as the status increases (Figure 4). For the educational status, the variation is still larger, ranging from 19.4% to 45% (p < 0.001).

Nevertheless, it is possible to indicate that even in the lower strata, the level of people outside the neighborhood and in the same city never falls below almost one fifth of the total of ties (19.4%). This argues against the thesis of total isolation of the people with fewer resources due to the effect of the neighborhood.

The variables of age and gender show smaller variations in the patterns of geographic location in comparison to the differences by status. In terms of age, the maximum of personal ties outside the neighborhood (more than 2km and less than 50km) is registered in the population between 36 and 55 years, and the minimum appears in the category of 56 years and above (38% and 24.5% respectively, p < 0.001).

According to gender, the differences for relations within the same city are smaller but yet significant, showing men with 35.6% of their personal ties outside the 2km range and women 29.3% (p < 0.01).

Figure 4. Personal ties distribution for geographical distance to the alteris’ house by educational status, economic status, age and gender. Argentina, 2006 (selected cities).

Distance to the house
(% per row)

Up to 20 blocks (2km)

20 blocks to 50 km

More than 50 km

Less than 5 blocks

5 to 10 blocks

11 to 20 blocks

Total

Educational status

 

 

 

 

 

 

Low

48,8

17,7

7,4

74,0

19,4

6,6

Medium

35,3

18,4

12,4

66,1

29,1

4,8

High

22,0

12,5

14,7

49,2

45,0

5,8

Economic status

 

 

 

 

 

 

Low

48,6

13,0

9,1

70,7

25,4

3,9d

Medium

38,0

17,5

12,3

67,8

24,4

7,8d

High

21,2

17,0

13,5

51,6

43,4

5,0

Age

 

 

 

 

 

 

18-35

35,2

14,9

12,0

62,2

32,1

5,7

36-55

28,3

15,0

12,4

55,6

38,0

6,3

56+

39,9

20,2

11,1

71,1

24,5

4,4

Gender

 

 

 

 

 

 

Male

31,1

17,1

11,1

59,2

35,6

5,2

Female

36,8

15,2

12,7

64,7

29,3

6,1

Total

34,1

16,1

11,9

62,1

32,3

5,6

Source: Author’s calculations based on data from the Encuesta de la Deuda Social Argentina 2006.

All together

In Figure 5, a combination of geographical distance, origin and type of relation is shown. In the first column of every origin category (workplace, neighborhood, etc.) the number of personal ties for the category is shown, normalized to the total number of personal ties (% of total table). Next, the percentage of personal ties within the category that corresponds to someone living at less than 2km is calculated (‘< 2km %’).

We will first examine relations between geographical distance and type of relation, then between geographical distance and origin of the personal tie, and lastly between type of relation and origin.

About distance and type of relation (last column, named ‘total’) it’s worth noting that friend and boyfriend/girlfriend are the categories with higher values (excluding the neighbor category) for ‘less than 2km distance’: 68% of all boyfriend/girlfriend lived within the 2km perimeter, and 64.66% of friends were under similar circumstances. Family has about half of the personal ties declared within this distance (51.58%). 

Regarding distance and origin of the personal tie (last row, named ‘total’), indirect relations (people you have met through someone you knew) is the category (excluding neighborhood) with more ties within a 2km distance (60.56%). This expresses some level of transitivity of proximity when creating ties through  people who are known well: not only people you know who live close to your house, but who also introduce you to people who live nearby. Family and educationally created ties are less locally distributed, but still about 50% of them are located at less than 2km.

The combination of type of relation and origin of the personal tie enlightens the composition of specific categories: While in the case of friendship the origin more usual for relations is neighborhood (23.58% of total number of ties), more boyfriends and girlfriends usually meet in educational institutions (0.46% of total ties).

Figure 5. Origin of personal tie and participation of ties within 2km by type of relation. Argentina, 2006 (selected cities).


Origin of the
personal tie
(% of total table) / less than 2km
(% of cell)

Places

Referred

Direct

Other, NR

Total

Elementary,
high school or university

Workplace

Neighborhood

Through a friend, girlfriend, boyfriend or kin

a kin

total %

< 2km %

total %

< 2km %

total %

< 2km %

total %

< 2km %

total %

< 2km %

total %

< 2km %

total %

< 2km %

Type of relation

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Friend

15.92

48.71

6.27

40.47

23.58

86.65

10.35

60.55

0.85

73.90

3.72

43.67

60.69

64.66

Kin

0.33

46.26

0.05

 

0.22

10

1.59

54.04

22.16

51.02

0.04

10

24.38

51.58

Neighbor

 

 

 

 

4.16

97.73

0.57

10

0.10

10

0.37

10

5.26

98.21

Boyfriend/
Girlfriend

0.46

78.39

0.10

10

0.31

36.50

1.29

71.93

0.23

64.71

0.17

53.97

2.56

68.00

Coworker/ classmate

1.56

45.91

2.85

40.30

0.17

 

 

 

0.14

 

0.30

10

5.02

43.15

Professional services

 

 

0.14

 

 

 

0.41

7.86

 

 

0.29

40.67

0.85

19.03

Other

 

 

0.04

 

 

 

0.12

21.49

0.06

10

0.27

16.32

0.52

29.18

NR

 

 

0.15

 

 

 

 

 

0.33

72.01

0.24

 

0.72

32.98

Total

18.30

49.28

9.63

39.61

28.48

87.31

14.33

60.56

23.87

52.29

5.40

47.86

100

61.44

Source: Author’s calculations based on data from the Encuesta de la Deuda Social Argentina 2006.


V. Conclusions

First, it is important to stress that all findings in this paper are derived from the single name generator we used, i.e., when we say that the ‘personal ties’ or the ‘personal networks’ of our sample exhibits a certain trend or correlation, we are only trying to inform about the behavior of the specific type of relations our name generator could obtain. Networks of highly trusted relations can be selective by income while the networks of occasional contacts may not; educational status may play a role in this type of ties while it may not for family only contacts, and so on. However, we could learn from the data that beyond the features explicitly required by the name generator, the relations we came to elicit were in most of the cases long-standing relations (more than 5 years) and were relations where people would share conversations about ‘important personal matters’.

Second, not only were educational status and economic status shown to be effective at discriminating sociability behavior towards neighborhood, but also age and gender appeared as powerful explanatory variables of the phenomena. The personal relations of women were more associated with family and nearby persons than men’s relations. Likewise, the measurement of personal ties allowed quantifying these gender trends, indicating its currency, but also stating its limited force, i.e., even when women show more personal ties at neighborhood and family, they are far from being excluded from other circles of sociability. Many geographical distances and sources of personal ties are observed both for men and women.

The analysis of age groups showed that the relevance of the neighborhood increases as people grow older, suggesting that neighborhood relations ­–people declared as neighbors, ties originated in the neighborhood as well as people who live nearby– are more available or desirable for the elderly than other kinds of relations. It is unclear, however, whether this prevalence of the neighborhood relations is better explained on the basis of durability or availability. That is to say, whether this kind of tie is stronger and better preserved over time than other ties, or rather that it is easier for older people to create such a tie, or a combination of both effects.  

As for educational status and economic status, they showed similar trends all along the analysis, in terms of more relevance of neighborhood and more relevance of geographically close relations when approaching the lower strata. However, educational status consistently exhibited stronger differences of neighborhood significance between its lower and higher strata compared to economic status in all of the variables considered. It is possible that the specificity of educational status can be rooted in at least two factors. First, its experience based nature, i.e., that educational status is usually attained by long term learning processes that imply per se spending time with others (teachers, professors, students) thus having more impact in specific ways of establishing bonds. Second, educational status differs from economic status in that it implies more consistently the assimilation of symbolic elements that may affect the ability and interest to relate to certain people (e.g. people in the neighborhood). Education is directly related to the incorporation of manners, values andthe past experiences of others.  Furthermore, in the case of tertiary and university education, education is responsible for professional specialization, with all the social and personal singularities that such an experience may imply or promote. 

Consequently, the effects related to trying to keep in touch with people or places farther from the residence area (such as affording higher transportation costs, gaining access to more diverse educational contents, developing more complex professional careers) associate the possibility of doing it with higher levels of social status (greater availability of capital). This process can work in both directions as it does in canonical examples of the benefits of social capital’s diversity: higher economic status allows staying in touch with people outside the neighborhood, and those personal connections become relevant in job searches thus impacting future income levels. A similar reasoning can be developed for feedback between educational status and extra-neighborhood personal ties.

Nevertheless, the data presented in this article is far from identifying social closures (high segregation) between neighborhoods (such as ghettos), as, even at the lowest strata, about one fifth of the ties are outside the 2km range. This fact leads us to assert –for the set of cities investigated in Argentina– that it is both reasonable to recognize neighborhood as a key social space for sociability, and to visualize the relative freedom all people have shown to mobilize, create and maintain relations within the city but outside its neighborhood.

Finally, the thesis that the term ‘neighbor’ should not be used as the only way to map the neighborhood (i.e. my neighborhood is more than the set of my neighbors) has been confirmed and extended. Moreover, geographical distance to ties seemed also insufficient for characterizing the role of neighborhood in sociability. Many studies found in the literature review were concerned about the relation between social distance and geographical distance, and quite often they tried to understand how both levels relate in order to measure neighborhood personal ties by a combination, or an addition, of neighbor’s qualities. Our study, however, does not focus primarily in that direction. Even when the relation of personal ties and geographical distance can be examined (there is more proximity of ties on lower strata and at the same time smaller personal networks), we wanted to focus on demonstrating that past experiences, valuation of tie (through type of tie classification) and physical distance were not redundant levels of information. On the contrary, they only partially overlap, and the relevance of neighborhood should not be restricted to those overlapping features: Neighborhood can be recognized at many levels, all legitimate and expressive fields of local, urban experiences.



Notes:


1 In 2006, the survey was applied in greater Buenos Aires, greater Cordoba, greater Mendoza, Bahia Blanca, Neuquén-Plottier, greater Salta and greater Resistencia.

2 In the original Spanish: “Con frecuencia, la gente recurre a amigos, familiares, compañeros de trabajo o conocidos cuando necesitan un consejo o ayuda para situaciones que sin ellos serían difíciles de resolver. Entre sus conocidos, sin incluir a quienes viven en su hogar, dígame por favor, solamente el nombre de las personas a las que recurriría en este tipo de situaciones

3 For a discussion of the technique of name generators and household surveys, see Burt, 1984.

4 The ‘equivalent adult’ is a coefficient that represents how many people live in a household, weighting the people by gender and age after their nutritional expected needs. This coefficient takes as a standard unit the nutritional needs of an adult male between 30 and 59 years (Salvia, 2001:258). The total number of members in each house was adjusted to this coefficient to normalize income by the size of the household.

5 Source: Author’s calculations based on data from the Censo Nacional de Población, Hogares y Viviendas (INDEC, 2001).

6 2km = 1.25 miles.

7 50km = 31.25 miles.

 

References

Agneessens, F., Waegea, H., Lievensa, J., 2006. Diversity in social support by role relations: A typology. Social Networks 28 (4), 427-441.

Ainsworth J., 2002. Why Does It Take a Village? The Mediation of Neighborhood Effects on Educational Achievement. Social Forces, Vol 81 (1), 117-152.

Atria R, Siles M., Arriagada I., Robison L., Whiteford S. (comps)., 2003. Capital social y reducción de la pobreza en América Latina y el Caribe: en busca de un nuevo paradigma. CEPAL, Santiago de Chile.

Bagnasco, A., Piselli, F., Pizzorno, A., Trigilia, C., 2003. El capital social. Instrucciones de uso. Fondo de Cultural Económico, Buenos Aires.

Bourdieu, P., 1994. Distinction: A Social Critique of the Judgement of Taste. Harvard University Press, Cambridge (USA).

Burt, R., 1984. Network Items and the General Social Survey. Social Networks 6, 293-339.

Burt, R., 2000. The social capital of structural holes. Cap 7. En New Directions in Economic Sociology. Russell Sage, Nueva York.

Castro, R., Campero, L., Hernández, B., 1997. La investigación sobre apoyo social en salud: situación actual y nuevos desafíos. Rev. Saúde Pública 31 (4), 425-35.

CEPAL, 2001. Panorama Social de América Latina. CEPAL, Santiago de Chile.

CEPAL / CELADE, 2002. “Vulnerabilidad Sociodemográfica: viejos y nuevos riesgos para comunidades, hogares y personas. Síntesis y Conclusiones”. LC/6.2170 (SES.29/16). Santiago de Chile.

Cortese, C., Falk F., Cohen, J., 1976. Further considerations on the methodological analysis of segregation indices. American Sociological Review 41.

Daraganova G., Pattison P., Koskinen J., Mitchell B., Bill A., Watts M., Baumd S., 2012. Networks and geography: Modelling community network structures as the outcome of both spatial and network processes. Social Networks 34, 6-17

Dawkins, C., 2004. Measuring the Spatial Pattern of Residential Segregation. Urban Studies 41.

De Federico de la Rúa, A., 2003. La dinámica de las redes de amistad: La elección de amigos en el programa Erasmus. Revista Redes 4.

De Filippis, J., 2001. The Myth of Social Capital in Community Development, Housing Policy Debate 12 (4).

De Grande, P., Eguia M., 2008. Reconstruyendo la red de lazos personales. Metodología egocéntrica para investigación sociocéntrica. Redes, Vol.15 (9).

Degenné, A., Forsé, M., 1999. Personal networks nd local circles. In Introducing Social Networks, Cap. 2. Sage Publications, London.

Doreian, P., Conti, N., 2012. Social context, spatial structure and social network structure. Social Networks 34, 32-46
.
Duncan, O., Duncan, B., 1955. A Methodological Analysis of Segregation Indexes. American Sociological Review 20.

Durkheim, E., 1984 [1893]. The Division of Labour in Society. The Macmillan Press, London.

Engels, F., 1987 [1845]. The condition of the working class in England. Penguin, London.

Enriquez Rosas R., 2000. Dinámica de las redes sociales y de apoyo emocional en hogares pobres urbanos el caso de México. Meeting of the Latin American Studies Association, Hyatt, Regency Miami, March 16-18.

Espinoza V., 1999. Social Networks Among the Urban Pool: Inequality and Integration in a Latin American City. In Wellman, B. (Ed.) Networks in the Global Village. Life in Contemporary Communities. Westview Press, Boulder (USA), pp. 149-184.

Feldman, S., Murmis, M., 2002. Las ocupaciones informales y sus formas de sociabilidad: apicultores, albañiles y feriantes. In L. Beccaria, S. Feldman, I. González Bombal, G. Kessler, M. Murmis, M. Svampa (Eds.), Sociedad y Sociabilidad en la Argentina de los 90. Biblos, Buenos Aires, pp. 173-230.

Ferrand A., Mounier L., Degenne A., 1999. The Diversity of Personal Networks in France: Social Stratification and Relational Structures. In Wellman, B. (Ed.) Networks in the Global Village. Life in Contemporary Communities. Westview Press, Boulder (USA), pp. 185-224.

Fischer, C., 1982. To Dwell among Friends: Personal Networks in Town and City, Londres: University of Chicago Press.

Forni, P., Nardone, M., 2005. Grupos solidarios de microcrédito y redes sociales: sus implicancias en la generación de capital social en barrios del Gran Buenos Aires. Revista Redes 9 (5), 1-25.

Gencoz, T., Ozlale, Y., 2004. Direct and indirect effects of social support on psychological well-being. Social Behavior and Personality 32 (5), 449-458.

Goodman, A., 1985. A Note on Neighborhood Size and the Measurement of Segregation Indices. Journal of Regional Science 25.

Granovetter, M., 1973. The strength of weak ties. American Journal of Sociology 78 (6), 1360-1380.

Gravano, A., 2003. Antropología de lo barrial. Estudios sobre producción simbólica de la vida urbana. Espacio Editorial, Buenos Aires.

Gravano, A., 2005. El Barrio en la Teoría Social. Espacio Editorial, Buenos Aires.

Groisman, F., Suárez, L., 2005. Segregación urbana en el Gran Buenos Aires. Paper presented at the IV Jornada sobre Mercado de Trabajo y Equidad en Argentina. 1-2 december. UNGS, Los polvorines.

Groisman, F., Suárez, L., 2006. “Nuevos desarrollos en la segregación residencial en el Gran Buenos”, V Jornada sobre Mercado de Trabajo y Equidad en Argentina. 30 november - 1 december. UNGS, Los polvorines.

Grootaert, C., 1998. ‘Social Capital: The Missing Link?’. Social Capital Initiative, Working Paper Series, No. 3. The World Bank Social, Washington DC.

Grossetti, M., 2005. Where do social relations come from? A study of personal. networks in the Toulouse area of France. Social Networks 27, 289-300.

Hipp J., Faris R., Boessena A., 2012. Measuring ‘neighborhood’: Constructing network neighborhoods. Social Networks 34, 128-140.

House, J., Umberson, D., Landis, K., 1988. Structures and Processes of Social Support. Annual Review of Sociology 14, 293-318.

INDEC, 2001. Censo Nacional de Población, Hogares y Viviendas 2001 (CD-ROM).

Ioannides, Y., Zabel, J., 2008. Interactions, neighborhood selection and housing demand. Journal of Urban Economics, 63, 229-252.

Jenks, Ch., Mayer, S., 1990. The social consequences of growing up in a poor neighborhood. In Lynn, L., McGeary, M. (coomp.): Inner City Poverty in the United States. National Academy Press, Washington D.C.

Katzman, R., 1999. Vulnerabilidad, activos y exclusión social en Argentina y Uruguay. OIT, Santiago de Chile.

Kaztman, R., 2001. Seducidos y abandonados: El aislamiento social de los pobres urbanos. Revista de la CEPAL 75, 171-189.

Kaztman, R., Retamoso, A., 2005. Segregación espacial, empleo y pobreza en Montevideo. Revista de la CEPAL 85, 131-148, Abril, México.

Kenneth, A., Strudler, B., De Vellis, R., 1978. Development of the multidimensional health locus of control scales. Health Education, Behavior 6 (1), 160-170.

Latané, B., Liu, J., Nowak, A., Bonevento, M., Zheng, L., 1995. Distance matters: physical space and social impact. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 21, 795-805.

Lederman, D., 2001. Socializing in Argentina: Levels, Geographic Distribution and Determinants of Social Capital. World Bank.

Lee R., Ruan D., Lai G., 2005. Social structure and support networks in Beijing and Hong Kong. Social Networks 27. 249-274.

Lieber, C., Sandefur, G., 1998. Exchanging Social Support with Friends, Neighbors, and Coworkers. CDE Working Paper No. 98-19. pp. 1-38.

Lin, N., 2001. Social capital: a theory of social structure and action. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

Lin, N., Simeone, R., Ensel, W., Kuo, W., 1979. Social support, stressful life events, and illness: A model and an empirical test. Journal of Health and Social 20, 108-119.

Martínez García, M., García Ramirez, M., Maya Jariego, I., 2002. Social support and locus of control as predictors of psychological well-being in Moroccan and Peruvian immigrant women in Spain. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 26, 287-310.

Massey, D., Denton, N., 1988. The dimension of residential segregation. Social Forces 67 (2), December.

Maya Jariego, I., Holgado, D., 2005. Lazos fuertes y proveedores múltiples de apoyo: comparación de dos formas de representación gráfica de las redes personales. EMPIRIA. Revista de Metodología de Ciencias Sociales 10, 107-127.

McCarty, C., Bernard, H., Killworth, P., Johnsen E., Shelley G., 1997. Eliciting Representative Samples of Personal Networks. Social Networks, 19, 303-323.

Mcpherson, M., Lovin, L., Cook, J., 2001. Birds of a Feather: Homophily in Social Networks. Annual Review of Sociology 27 (1), 415-444.

Mickelson K., Kubzansky L., 2003. Social Distribution of Social Support: The Mediating Role of Life Events. American Journal of Community Psychology 32.

Molina, J., 2005. El estudio de las redes personales: contribuciones, métodos y perspectivas. Empiria 10, 71-105.

Molina, J., Miranda, L., Lozares, C., 2012. The  Geographical  Distribution  of  the  Personal  Networks  of  People   Living  in  Catalonia:  a  dual  society. Grafo Working Papers, 1 (1-19)

Morgan, B., Norbury, J., 1981. Some Further Observations on the Index of Residential Differentiation. Demography 18, May.

ODSA, 2007. Barómetro de la Deuda Social Argentina / 3. Progresos Sociales 2004-2006. Buenos Aires: Fundación Arcor y EDUCA. Available at: http://www.uca.edu.ar/uca/common/grupo68/files/Barometro_2007_-_Completo.pdf).

ONS, 2001. Social Capital: A Review of Literature. Office of the National Statistics, UK.

Park, R.E., Burgess, E.W., McKenzie, R.D., 1925. The City. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

PNUD (Programa de las Naciones Unidas para el Desarrollo), 1998. El capital social hacia la construcción del índice de desarrollo sociedad civil de Argentina, PNUD, Buenos Aires.

Preciado, P, Snijders, T., Burk, W., Stattin H., Kerr M., 2012. Does proximity matter? Distance dependence of adolescent friendships. Social Networks 34, 18-31.

Rodríguez, J., Arraigada, C., 2004. Segregación residencial en la ciudad latinoamericana,  EURE 29, N°89. Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago de Chile.

Sabaté, A., 2000. Economía y Sociedad de la región metropolitana de Buenos aires en el contexto de la reestructuración de los 90.  Paper presented by Sabaté y J. L. Coraggio in the Seminario Internacional Las Grandes Regiones Metropolitanas del Mercosur y México: entre la competitividad y la complementariedad, organized by IC - UNGS, 28-30 november 2000, Buenos Aires. 

Sabatini F., Cáceres G., Cerda, J., 2001. Segregación residencial en las principales ciudades chilenas: Tendencias de las tres últimas décadas y posibles cursos de acción. EURE, 27, 82, Santiago de Chile.

Sabatini, F., 2003. On Ben Fine Social Capital versus Social Theory: Political Economy and Social Science at the Turn of the Millennium, Economic Notes 32 (3).

Sabatini, F., 2008. Social Capital as Social Networks: a New Framework for Measurement and an empirical analysis of its determinants and consequences, Journal of Socio-Economics 38 (3), 429-442.

Salvia, A., De Grande, P., 2007. Segregación residencial socioeconómica y espacio social: deserción escolar de los jóvenes en el área metropolitana del gran buenos aires. XXVI Congreso Asociación Latinoamericana de Sociología. 13-18 August, Guadalajara, México.

Salvia, A., 2001. Bienestar económico y desigualdad social en los hogares del Gran Buenos Aires durante la política neoliberal. In Gómez, C. (Eds.) Procesos Sociales, Población y Familia. Alternativas teóricas y empíricas en las investigaciones sobre la vida domestica. Ed. Plaza y Valdes, México. pp. 255-278.

Schaefer, D., 2012. Youth co-offending networks: An investigation of social and spatial effects. Social Networks 34, 141- 149.
Schwirian, K., 1983. Models of neighborhood change. Annual Review of Sociology 9, 83-102.

Simmel, G., 1949 [1910]. The Sociology of Sociability. The American Journal of Sociology 55, No. 3, 254-261.

Stevens, N., Van Tilburg, T., 2011. Cohort differences in having and retaining friends in personal networks in later life. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 28(1), 24-43.

Szwarcberg, M., 2012. Revisiting clientelism: A network analysis of problem-solving networks in Argentina. Social Networks 34, 230-240.

Torres, H., 2009. Procesos recientes de fragmentación socio-espacial en Buenos Aires: la suburbanización de las élites. In Pírez, P. (Ed.) Buenos Aires, la formación del presente. OLACCHI, Quito.

Van der Gaag, M., 2005. Measurement of Individual Social Capital. University of Groningen and Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam.

Van der Poel, M., 1993. Delineating personal support networks. Social Networks, 15(1), 49-70.

Wellman, B., Potter, S., 1999. The elements of personal communities. In Wellman, B. (Ed.) Networks in the Global Village. Life in Contemporary Communities. Westview Press, Boulder (USA), pp. 49-81.

White, M., 1984. The Measurement of Spatial Segregation. American Journal of Sociology 88.

Wilson, W., 1987. The Truly Disadvantaged; The Inner City, the Underclass, and Public Policy. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

Wirth, L., 1928. The Ghetto. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

Wong, D., 2002. Modeling Local Segregation A Spatial Interaction Approach. Geographical and Environmental Modeling 6, No. 1.

Woolcock, M., 2001. The place of social capital in Understanding Social and Economic Outcomes. ISUMA: Canadian Journal of Policy Research 2 (1), 1-17.

Zuckerman, A., 2005. The Social Logic of Politics: Personal Networks as Contexts for Political Behavior. Temple University Press, Filadelfia.

Creative Commons License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License.

Last updated December 31, 2014